Fish Are Animals Who Do Things
If you find it hard to believe that fish feel pain, consider this!
Cognition—the ability to think—was an important factor in the
establishment of whether fish could feel pain, and it happened that
in the same year as Rose's article was published, Bshary, Wickler,
and Fricke published a review of the findings on the cognitive
capabilities of fish (Bshary et al 2002). Here are a few examples:
Recognition
of others as individuals has long been established in many varieties
of fish, both visually and acoustically (Myrberg and Riggio 1985). It
forms the first step towards complex social lives, in which cognition
is often most evident. I documented relationships among reef sharks
for years. They, too, relate to each other individually.
Social
learning is illustrated by the migrations of the surgeon fish
(Acathurus nigrofuscus), described in detail by Arthur A.
Myrberg Jr. in 1998. These fish leave their territories all over the
lagoon, and travel in single file through paths in the coral to their
traditional spawning grounds. They go and return along the same paths
each night at precisely the same time, as I saw in the local lagoon,
year after year. Their spawning ground was the only place along the
lagoon's border where the outflowing current was exactly balanced by
the incoming surge, so that the huge cloud of spawn that they left in
the gathering night, stayed in place. These short term migrations had
been shown to be the result of social learning; each generation of
fish learned from its elders where to go to spawn, and when.
Triggerfish
(Balistidae) often feed on sea urchins. Usually, they try to
‘blow’
them onto their side to get access to the unprotected body parts
beneath. Hans Fricke (1971) observed at Eilat how five different
individuals of Balistapus undulatus sucessfully hunted sea
urchins by first biting off the spines, which allowed them to grab
the urchin and take it to the surface. They fed on the unprotected
parts underneath while the urchin slowly sank. In spite of decades of
observations, Fricke never saw this behaviour anywhere else. It
appeared to be the result of social learning.
Intertidal
gobies (Gobius soporator), live in tide pools, and during low
tide they can jump from one to another, without being able to see
their target pool at the beginning of the jump. Experimentation
showed that the fish had memorized the lay of the land around the
home pool by swimming over it when the tide was in, so were referring
to a three dimensional memory to navigate when the outgoing tide left
them only a labyrinth of pools. (Aronson
1951, 1956).
With the
exception of humans, fish are more skillful than primates at nest
building. At least 9000 fish species build some sort of nest, either
for egg laying or for protection.
The male
minnow Exoglossum selects more than three hundred stones, all of the
same size, from over 5 m distance, to build a spawning mound 35 cm
wide and 10 cm high (van Duzer 1939). Another fish builds dome-shaped
nests from 10,000 pebbles (Lachner 1952).
The
jawfish, (Opistognathus aurifrons), collects stones of various
sizes to build a wall, leaving a hole just big enough to pass
through. This involves repeated rearrangement of the stones. In
between, the fish searches for new stones that might better fit the
available space than the ones it has already collected, using
flexible behaviour depending on the circumstances (Kacher 1963; Colin
1972, 1973).
Another
example showing surprising flexibility of behaviour is the ability of
the ten-spined stickleback (Pygosteus pungitius), to build his
nest around the eggs if the female has already laid them, though he
usually builds the nest first (Leiner 1931). Great care is required,
and a different technique has to be used to avoid damaging the eggs.
Since those eggs do hatch, the males achieve their goal. (Morris
1958)
Redouin
Bshary (2002), described seeing cooperative hunting between red sea
coral groupers (Plectropomus pessuliferus), or lunartail
groupers (Variola louti), and giant moray eels (Gymnothorax
javanicus):
“These two large species of groupers were observed regularly approaching the eels that were resting in a coral cave, and shaking their bodies in exaggerated movements, usually at less than 1 m distance to the moray eel.... In 7 of 14 observations, the moray eel left its cave and the two predators would swim next to each other, searching for prey. The groupers would often come so close that the two predators touched each other at their sides. While the moray eels sneaked through holes, the groupers waited above the corals for escaping fish.”
Another
unusual form of cooperation among different species is seen in
cleaning symbiosis. Cleaner fish come from many different fish
families, and depend on cleaning for their diet to varying degrees.
They clean the dead skin and ectoparasites from their clients in
return for a meal. Full time cleaners may have about 2,300
interactions per day, with clients belonging to 100 different
species! (Grutter 1995)
According
to the evidence, cleaners have their hundred client species
categorized as those who only come to their local cleaner, and those
whose home ranges include the territories of other cleaners. For the
latter, they have competition, so give them priority over the locals,
who have no choice of cleaner.
Cleaners
sometimes “cheated” by feeding off the client's healthy flesh as
well as doing the usual cleaning job, and the clients with no choice
of cleaner punished the cleaner by aggressively chasing it, and
inflicting a bite or two, as they saw fit. (Clutten-Brock and Parker
1995). But these clients benefited in the future, because the cleaner
fish were seen to give them, but not others who visited in the
meantime, a better-than-average cleaning service on the next visit!
Apparently cleaners can distinguish more than 100 individual clients
belonging to various species.
Cleaners
will hover above the client and touch it with their fins, in an
effort to influence its decision to come for a cleaning. This
touching tactic is also used to try to reconcile with a client whom
they have cheated as described above. Cleaners even exploit the
presence of a third party in an attempt to make aggressive clients
stop chasing them by going to a nearby predator and caressing it, so
that the client dares not continue the chase!
Cleaners
will behave altruistically toward their clients if they are being
watched by potential new clients—but only those who could visit
another cleaning station. Since clients will emulate the behaviour of
the former client, the sight of another being treated very well by
the cleaner, is more likely to convince it to come for servicing than
seeing another client being chased or eaten! This tactic suggests a
short term image, or social prestige (Alexander 1987; Zahavi 1995;
Nowak and Sigmund 1998; Roberts 1998), that determines their success
in attracting new clients.
Such
complex social behaviour—cheating, reconciliation, altruism,
species recognition, individual recognition, punishment, social
prestige, and bookkeeping, displayed by full-time cleaners 50 to 100
times per day, is generally considered to indicate consciousness when
displayed in primates.
A similar
example of social judgement is given by predator inspection, in which
different individuals take turns to lead others away from the school
to look over a predator. Fish who don't take their turn
cooperatively, will not be trusted by their partners in the future.
In other words, the fish make an evaluation of the behaviour of
another individual, remembers it, and takes it into account in future
decisions. (Milinski et al. 1990b; Dugatkin and Wilson 1993)
At the
end of the review of cognition in fish, Bshary wrote: “We are aware
of only one experimentally shown qualitative difference in mechanisms
between primates and fish, and this difference is the ability to
imitate.”
References
Alexander
RD (1987) The biology of moral systems. Aldine de Gruiter, New York
Aronson
LR (1951) Orientation and jumping behavior in the goby fish
Bathygobius soporator. Am
Mus Novitates 1486
Aronson
LR (1956) Further studies on orientation and jumping behaviour in
the goby fish Bathygobius soporator. Anat
Rec 125:606
Bshary
R, Wickler W, Fricke H (2002) Fish cognition: a primate's eye view.
Animal Cognition (2002) 5 : 1-13
Colin
PL (1972) Daily activity patterns and effects of environmental
conditions on the behaviour of the yellowhead jawfish Opistognathus
aurifrons, with notes on its ecology. Zoologica 57:137-169
Colin
PL (1973) Burrowing behaviour of the yellowhead jawfish,
Opistognathus aurifrons. Copeia
1973:84-90
Barber
TX (1993) The Human Nature of Birds. Bookman Press,
Melbourne
Clutten-Brock TH,
Parker GA (1995) Punishment in animal societies. Nature 373:209-215
Chandroo
KP, Yue S, and Mocci RD (2004) An evaluation of current perspectives
on consciousness and pain in fishes. Fish and Fisheries 5. 281-295
Chapman,
CR and Nakamura Y (1999) A passion of the soul: an introduction to
pain for consciousness researchers. Consciousness and Cognition 8.
391-422
Dugatkin
LA, Wilson DS (1993) Fish behaviour, partner choice, and cognitive
ethology. Rev Fish Biol Fish 3:368-372
Duzer
EM van (1939) Observations on the breeding habits of the cut-lip
minnow, Exoglossum maxillingua. Copeia 1939:65-75
Fricke
H (1971) Fische als Feinde tropischer Seeigel. Mar Biol 9:328-338
Edelman GM and
Tononi G (2000)A Universe of Consciousness. Basic Books, New York,
NY
Glynn I. (1999) An
Anatomy of Thought: The Origin and Machinery of the Mind. Wiedenfield
and Nicholson
Geiger W (1956)
Quantitative Untersuchungen über das Gehirn der Knochenfische, mit
besonderer Berücksichtigung seines relativen Wachstums, I Acta Anat
26: 121-163
Grandin
T. and Deesing M. American Board of Veterinary Practitioners -
Symposium 2002 May 17, 2002, Special Session Pain, Stress, Distress
and Fear. Emerging Concepts and Strategies in Veterinary Medicine
Griffin
DR (1998) Animal Minds. The University of Chicago Press.
Grutter
AS 1995 Relationships between cleaning rates and ectoparasite loads
in coral reef fishes. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 118:51-58
Harmes
CA, Lewbart GA, Swanson CR, Kishimori JM, Boylan SM (2005)
Behavioural and Clinical Pathology Changes in Koi Carp (Cyprinus
carpio) Subjected to Anaesthesia
and Surgery with and without Intra-Operative Analgesics. Comparative
Medicine Vol. 55 No. 3 221-226
Kacher
H (1963) Opithognathus aurifrons (Opisthognathidae) Graben
einer Wohnhöhle, Wallbau Film E514, Encyclopaedia Cinematographica.
Institut für den wissenschaftlichen Film, Göttingen
Lachner
A (1952) Studies of the biology of the cyprinid fish of the chub
genus Nocomis of northeastern United States. Am Midl Nat 48:433-466
Laureys
S., Faymonville ME, Luxon A, Lamy M, Franck G, and Maquet P (2000)
Restoration of thalamocortical connectivity after recovery from
persistent
Lovibond
PF , Shanks DR (2002) The role of awareness in Pavlovian
conditioning: empirical evidence and theoretical implication.
Journal of Experimental Psychology 28 3-26
Maren
S (2001) Neurobiology of Pavlovian fear conditioning. Annual Review
of Neuroscience 24. 897-931
Marino
L (2002) Convergence of complex cognitive abilities in cetaceans and
primates. Brain, Behaviour and Evolution 59. 21-32
Milinski
M, Pfluger D, Külling D, Kettler R (1990b) Do sticklebacks
cooperate repeatedly in reciprocal pairs? Behav Ecol Sociobiol
27:17-21
Morris
D (1958) The reproductive behaviour of the ten-spined stickleback
(Pygosteus pungitius L.). Behaviour [Suppl] 6:1-154
Myrberg AA,
Riggio RJ (1985) Acoustically mediated individual recognition by a
coral reef fish (Pomacentrus portitus)
Myrberg AA,
Montgomery WL, Fishelson L (1988) The reproductive behaviour of
Acanthurus nigrofuscus (Forskal) and other surgeon fishes
(Fam. Acanthuridae) of Eilat, Israel (Gulf of Aqaba, Red Sea).
Ethology 79:31-61
Nowak MA, Sigmund
K (1998) Evolution of indirect reciprocity by image scoring. Nature
393:573-577
Overmier JB,
Hollis KL (1990) Fish in the think tank: learning, memory, and
integrated behaviour. Neurobiology of Comparative Cognition (eds
Kesner RP, Olson DS), Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsday, NJ. pp. 205-236
Roberts G (1998)
Competitive altruism: from reciprocity to the handicap principle.
Proc R Soc Lond B 265:427-431
Rose,
JD 2002 The neurobehavioural nature of fishes, and the question of
awareness and pain. Rev. Fish. Sci. 10:1-38
Sneddon, LU 2003.
The evidence for pain in fish: The use of morphine as an analgesic.
Applied Animal Behavior Science. 83:153-162.
Zahavi A (1995)
Altruism as a handicap—the limitations of kin selection and
reciprocity. J Avian Biol 26:1-3
Comments